Sunday, December 5, 2010

The Spotlight is NOT Always On ME !

From Los Angeles Times:
It's not all about you

Chances are, others aren't judging you as harshly as you think, if at all.

By Benedict Carey

Times Staff Writer




Oh, things sure took a bad turn. Mortifying, that's what it was. Such a big party -- friends, co-workers -- and you dumped that drink! How can you live with being such a klutz? Who there will ever forget it?


Take a deep breath. Stop obsessing. It probably wasn't as bad as you think. Not nearly.


A growing body of research shows that far fewer people notice our gaffes than we believe as we pace the floor in private, going over and over the faux pas. And those who do notice judge us less harshly than we imagine. In a series of groundbreaking studies over the last two years, psychologists have shown that the "spotlight effect," as they call it, is a universal experience that distorts our egocentric notion about the degree to which people in groups, like parties and work gatherings, pay attention to us.


Learning to recognize this self-deception can soothe the anxiety that surrounds social interactions. "In this case, the truth will set you free," said Kenneth Savitsky, a psychologist at Williams College in Williamstown, Mass., who studies how egocentrism affects behavior. "You can't completely eliminate the embarrassment you feel when you commit a faux pas, but it helps to know how much you're exaggerating its impact."


The spotlight effect blinds us in several ways. A few years ago, researchers at Cornell University conducted an experiment with 109 college students in which young men and women enter a roomful of their peers, alone, while wearing a Barry Manilow T-shirt. The pop singer wasn't exactly a favorite in the dorms of Ithaca, N.Y., at the time. The students felt self-conscious about the shirt, and after spending only moments in the room, met individually with researchers and guessed that at least half of their peers had noticed and might have said something about the Manilow shirt. Not so, the researchers found. On average, less than a quarter of the people in the room had paid any attention at all. Follow-up experiments using T-shirts have found that people exaggerate by up to six times the percentage of observers who notice.


A pioneer in this field, Tom Gilovich, a psychologist at Cornell, has demonstrated the same exaggerated misperceptions in several situations, such as group discussions about social issues. In a 2000 study, Gilovich and colleagues reported that students also badly overestimated how well their own gaffes and clever arguments were noticed by others in discussion groups. "The fact is that others do not notice us nearly as much as we think they do," Gilovich said. Contrary to every instinct, our nervousness, our sadness, even our lies are largely lost on most observers, he said.


The findings apply to most of us, of course, but not to everybody -- some people really do live under a microscope, as a chosen way of life. If the company's chief executive is cavorting with the bellhop at the annual retreat, she's surely going to hear about it. But as for the rest of us, our self-absorption not only creates a false spotlight, it also results in an exaggeration about how we are judged.


Most of the time a mistake is just a mistake, not a death sentence. As psychologists have shown, there is a knee-jerk critic in us all: When we see someone fall to the pavement, we may think, klutz; when a driver turns the wrong way down a one-way street, we may think, idiot. But the harshness is short-lived: Almost immediately, we tend to moderate our initial judgments if we continue to think about the incident at all, taking into account the circumstances: The road was icy, the one-way sign was obscured by a tree.


Yet we don't expect that same empathy for ourselves. In one recent experiment, psychologist Nicholas Epley of Harvard University and Gilovich asked students to imagine that they spilled a drink in their laps in the middle of an interview -- a blunder that went unnoticed until the interviewee stood to leave.


The researchers divided the young men and women into three groups. One group was asked to anticipate how the blunder would be evaluated by a harsh critic; another predicted how it would be viewed by a charitable person; and the third estimated how it would come across to an "average" interviewer. The students' responses showed they made no distinction between a harsh critic and an average one (they were all thought to be harsh), although they did expect to be judged much more positively by a charitable person, as the ratings showed. But that charitable person often doesn't come to mind. "This is a very reliable effect," said Epley. "When anticipating how others will evaluate us for our embarrassing mishaps, people seem to automatically imagine critics with horns and fangs."


Mark Leary, a psychology professor at Wake Forest University in Winston-Salem, N.C., said the spotlight effect, painful as it may be, has social utility, at least at the extremes. It's better to be too sensitive to what people may be thinking, even if you're wrong, than to be unconscious of it. "If you're going to err, you're better off being oversensitive when people aren't watching you than oblivious when they are," he said. "The risk of being excluded is too high, in terms of getting jobs, in terms of finding a lover, of being accepted socially."


Some researchers speculate that the habits date back to early human history, when people lived in small, highly interdependent groups. "In those societies, certainly, the price for being ostracized would be evolutionary death," Epley said. "You would have to be very attuned to how others viewed you."


In modern life, it nevertheless can be enormously helpful to cut yourself a break. In a report due out this year, psychologists find for the first time that simple awareness of this native oversensitivity can improve how people do when they actually are in the spotlight. Savitsky and Gilovich had 77 Cornell students make a three-minute public speech on university race relations. The speakers had five minutes to prepare. Half were told in a vague sort of way not to worry, that it's natural to be anxious about public speaking. The other half were offered some genuine education on printed materials. They read about specific psychological findings that speakers "feel that their nervousness is transparent, but in reality their feelings are not so apparent to observers. If you become nervous, you'll probably be the only one to know." The result: The better-informed speakers were significantly more lucid and less nervous, as rated by outside judges. The researchers have not yet studied how well speakers do when they're nervous -- and have dumped a drink in their lap.

2 comments:

Gary Kelly said...

Yep, I think many of us can be our own worst enemy, and judge ourselves far too harshly.

Of course, some people go the other way and don't judge themselves harshly enough.

This is where good friends - honest friends - come in handy as a sounding board.

jimm said...

Hmmm... where to start? I've made plenty of mistakes cuz of my poor hearing. That does draw the dreaded spotlight. Not to mention the ones who would stare at my aids.

So, as a teen, i let my hair long to cover my bte hearing-aids. But then ppl couldn't figure out why my speech was all mumbled.Results, more attention, for all the wrong reasons! ppl would judge me as stupid or ignorant or unfriendly.

Regardless, it was many years afore I kept those long locks trimmed back. So much for stigma, huh?

It's better to jus be myself. Accept the hearing loss (stigma and all)as part of who I am. And, Justin, no one taught me this as a youngster. Had to figure it all out myself.